Within science circles, trying to come up with a new universal language was a trendy past-time in the 17th Century. Even the man who discovered gravity, Sir Isaac Newton, took a stab at it. Arika Okrent, editor-at-large at TheWeek.com, talks about its failure to catch on with Weekends on All Things Considered host Jacki Lyden.
Copyright ? 2013 NPR. For personal, noncommercial use only. See Terms of Use. For other uses, prior permission required.
JACKI LYDEN, HOST:
Communications barriers have long vexed us as showcased in the movie "Rush Hour."
(SOUNDBITE OF MOVIE, "RUSH HOUR")
CHRIS TUCKER: (as Carter) Do you understand the words that are coming out of my mouth?
LYDEN: Scientists in the 17th century were working hard to understand mainly the secrets of the universe but also each other. With Latin on the decline, they were seeking a whole new way of communicating that would defy barriers and borders, a universal language. And though he's better known for discovering gravity, even Sir Isaac Newton took a stab at it. We know this because Newton left behind an outline of this new universal language in an old notebook.
Arika Okrent is an editor-at-large at theweek.com, and she's also a linguist who explains how this language would work.
ARIKA OKRENT: What Newton tried out was you didn't have to have different words for every degree of something. You could have one root. And the example he gives is tor for temperature. And then to make the word cold, you just add a prefix to it. And to make the word hot, you add a different prefix to it, and then you have different prefixes all the way through the whole scale of coldness to hotness. So utor is hot. Owtor is exceedingly hot. Etor is warm. Oytor is excessively cold and everything in between.
So you could have a degree of precision of temperature just by adding these set prefixes to that one concept.
LYDEN: So have you tried to speak in Sir Isaac Newton's language?
OKRENT: Well, he doesn't give enough vocabulary for you to really say anything. He just gives a few examples. The rest of it is all an outline of how it could work. And I think that's where many people got tripped up on this idea. It sounds really nice. Break down the universe into concepts and make a mathematics out of that, and then you have to sit down and figure out the universe. And that part's a lot harder.
But a colleague of his, John Wilkins, a member of the Royal Society, actually did this and has a 600-page breakdown of vocabulary based on everything in the universe. It was very well known in its day, and no one ever really spoke it.
LYDEN: So why did this bid at trying to create this universal language fail?
OKRENT: Well, it's nice to think that we could overcome misunderstandings if we could be so precise that exactly what we wanted to say would come through and the person on the other end could decompose our meaning perfectly. There's no fussiness in there. But that isn't the way that we use language. The fussiness and ambiguity in language is actually very useful to us.
We go ahead, we start talking without really knowing where we're going. We work out our thoughts as we speak, and it's hard to do that in a language where you have to know your exact meaning before you can even say anything.
LYDEN: So Newtonian didn't count for, in Arabic, you say yanni a lot, and in English, whatever.
(LAUGHTER)
OKRENT: Right. You need the whatever. You want to be able to say it's hot or it's cold without specifying it's very little exceedingly hot.
LYDEN: You know, this whole attempt to create a new language, I mean, it's always a wonderful concept. I remember Esperanto was going to be the universal language in the '60s. Why don't universal languages catch on very well?
OKRENT: I mean, Esperanto is the most successful one of all time in the sense that it's not a universal language, but people actually still speak it. But they do within their own little community. And I think that's the real problem. We can't have a universal language because we don't have a universal community. And that's where languages live, between people.
LYDEN: That's Arika Okrent, editor-at-large for theweek.com and the author of "In the Land of Invented Languages." Thank you very much for being with us.
OKRENT: Thank you so much. This was fun.
Copyright ? 2013 NPR. All rights reserved. No quotes from the materials contained herein may be used in any media without attribution to NPR. This transcript is provided for personal, noncommercial use only, pursuant to our Terms of Use. Any other use requires NPR's prior permission. Visit our permissions page for further information.
NPR transcripts are created on a rush deadline by a contractor for NPR, and accuracy and availability may vary. This text may not be in its final form and may be updated or revised in the future. Please be aware that the authoritative record of NPR's programming is the audio.
Source: http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=185348917&ft=1&f=1007
kansas vs ohio state winning mega million numbers bruce weber google maps 8 bit mirror mirror texas relays meniscus
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.